In many species of birds females initiate
incubation prior to laying the last eggs in the clutch. This causes
the offspring to hatch asynchronously. Hatching asynchrony creates
a size hierarchy among the nestlings and frequently results in the mortality
of later hatching offspring. The “egg viability hypothesis” suggests
that this apparently maladaptive behavior is the result of a decline in
the viability of the first laid eggs if they are not incubated within a
few days after they are laid. Thus, to ensure that all the eggs in
a clutch hatch, the females are constrained to commence incubation prior
to completion of the clutch.
This study will test the egg viability
hypothesis in Barn Swallows. Hatching asynchrony in unmanipulated
nests will be evaluated relative to ambient temperatures during the egg
laying period. A positive correlation between temperature and hatching
asynchrony will support the egg viability hypothesis. An experiment
will be conducted in which females will be prevented from incubating eggs
prior to clutch completion. Hatching success among the eggs in these
nests will be evaluated, first laid eggs are expected to suffer reduced
hatching success under the egg viability hypothesis. In addition,
the reproductive success of these experimental nests will be compared to
unmanipulated control, experimentally synchronized nests, and experimentally
asynchronous nests. The egg viability hypothesis will be supported
if the greatest reproductive success is experienced by experimentally synchronized
nests.
Hatching asynchrony occurs when birds
begin to incubate their eggs before all of the eggs in the clutch have
been laid. When this occurs, the first laid eggs hatch before later
laid eggs, resulting in a size hierarchy within the brood. These
size hierarchies have variable effects. In some species, the last
hatched offspring always perish, either from starvation (inability to compete
for resources) or because the older offspring kill them (siblicide).
This phenomenon, observed in Pelicans (Pelecanus erythrorhynchos) (Evans
and McMahon 1987, Evans 1990) and Boobies (Sula sp.) (Drummond and Chavelas
1989, Anderson 1990, 1995), is known as obligate brood reduction.
In other species, the younger (smaller) offspring may survive, but experience
a much higher risk of mortality than do their older (larger) broodmates.
This phenomenon, which is common in many passerines (Strehl 1978, Clark
and Wilson 1981, Richter 1984, Magrath 1990, Stouffer and Power 1991, Ricklefs
1995, Amundsen and Slagsvold 1996) and in parrots (Stoleson and Beissinger
1997, Krebs 1999), is known as facultative brood reduction.
Outwardly, because of its deleterious
effect on younger offspring, hatching asynchrony seems maladaptive.
Thus, hatching asynchrony is of interest in understanding the evolution
of avian breeding strategies (Clark and Wilson 1981, Magrath 1990, Amundsen
and Slagsvold 1996). Fifteen different hypotheses have been presented
to explain why hatching asynchrony occurs (Magrath 1990). Several
of these propose that parents and older broodmates might benefit from the
mortality of younger brood mates (Clark and Wilson 1981, Drummond 1987,
Amundsen and Slagsvold 1996, Wiebe 1996, Aparicio 1997). These hypotheses
suggest that hatching asynchrony is, in itself, an adaptation. In
addition, hypotheses have been presented suggesting that ecological or
physiological constraints force some birds to hatch their broods asynchronously
(Magrath 1990, Stoleson and Beissinger 1999). In this case,
hatching asynchrony is not adaptive, but is the consequence of a trade-off
between selective pressures that favor early incubation and selective pressures
that favor synchronous hatching.
The egg viability hypothesis proposes
that synchronous hatching is otherwise adaptive, but, because the viability
of unincubated eggs declines with time, birds are forced to commence incubation
before all the eggs are laid. Temperature is the most important factor
determining the viability of unincubated eggs. Embryos are most susceptible
to exposure to high and moderate temperatures (Webb 1987). At cool
temperatures, freshly laid eggs remain in a state of stasis (O’Connor
1984); no development will occur in eggs maintained at temperatures below
27 oC (physiological zero) (Rol’nik 1970, White and Kinney 1974, Wilson
1991). Thus, freshly laid eggs maintained at temperatures below physiological
zero can remain viable for an extended period of time. Development
commences as soon as the eggs are warmed above physiological zero.
However, optimal development occurs at incubation temperatures, typically
36 oC and 38oC (Webb 1987, Rahn 1991). Thus, in temperate climates,
cool ambient temperatures allow the eggs to remain dormant until the onset
of incubation (Drent 1975, Ewert 1992). On the other hand, exposure
to temperatures above physiological zero but below normal incubation temperature
can cause abnormal development and embryo mortality (Wilson 1991, Deeming
and Ferguson 1992). As a result, in warm climates, birds may be constrained
to commence incubation early in order to maintain egg viability and increase
hatching success.
Only one study has been conducted to
test the egg viability hypothesis in passerine birds (perching birds) (Veiga
1992, Veiga and Vinuela 1993). However, among passerines, there is
a general correlation between hatching asynchrony and latitude (Clark
and Wilson 1981, Hussell 1985), which suggests that egg viability might
provide a strong selective pressure favoring hatching asynchrony.
For example, in House Sparrows (Passer domesticus), eggs left unincubated
for 3 or more days and exposed to temperatures above physiological zero
exhibited reduced hatching success compared to eggs that were incubated
earlier (Veiga 1992, Veiga and Vinuela 1993). However, that study failed
to show whether hatching asynchrony represented an adaptive trade-off between
selective pressure for early incubation and selective pressure for total
brood synchrony.
The current study will test the egg
viability hypothesis in the American Barn Swallow (Hirundo rustica erythrogaster).
In addition, egg manipulation experiments will be conducted to determine
whether observed (unmanipulated) levels of hatching asynchrony represent
an optimal trade-off between the need to incubate the eggs early and the
need to synchronize hatching. Barn Swallows in Texas represent an
ideal species in which to conduct these studies. First, Texas climate
varies from relatively cool in the spring to extremely hot in the summer.
Thus, the importance of egg viability in favoring hatching asynchrony should
vary as the season advances and ambient temperatures increase. Secondly,
observations of 361 nests at two colonies south of Commerce, Texas during
the summers of 1998 and 1999, suggest that hatching asynchrony is typical,
but variable in this species. Most birds lay 5 eggs, but begin incubation
after laying the 3rd or 4th eggs and hatching asynchrony is most pronounced
near the end of the breeding season when ambient temperatures are the highest.
The egg viability hypothesis makes
some predictions that can be rather easily tested. First, unincubated
eggs should become less viable with time. Second, if early incubation
is a response to this loss of viability, then loss of viability should
be detectable within the time frame normally required to complete a clutch.
To test these predictions, eggs will be removed from the nests of barn
swallows immediately after laying and before any incubation takes place
(prior to 10 AM). The removed eggs will be replaced with artificial
surrogate eggs in order to keep the parents attendant at the nest.
The real eggs will be sequestered into a holding box equipped with an electronic
thermometer recording daily maximum and minimum temperatures. Barn
Swallows lay one egg each morning until the clutch is complete, and each
of these eggs in experimental nests will be marked according to laying
sequence and sequestered into the holding box. Once egg laying has
ended, the surrogate eggs in the nest will be removed and the real eggs
will be placed back into the nest. As a result of this manipulation,
each egg will have been exposed to ambient temperatures for a different
number of days (ranging from 0 to 4). The egg viability hypothesis
predicts that the first laid eggs should suffer reduced hatching success
and that this reduced hatching success should be correlated with the extent
to which the eggs are exposed to ambient temperatures above physiological
zero. The manipulation will allow hatching success to be measured
and assessed relative to laying sequence and the level of exposure to ambient
temperatures above physiological zero. Hatching success will also
be compared to that of control nests.
The egg viability hypothesis suggests
that hatching asynchrony is imposed by a physiological constraint and represents
a potential cost to the parents in terms of lowered reproductive success.
Thus, it is also predicted that experimentally synchronized nests should
have higher reproductive success than control nests, nests for which eggs
have been sequestered, and completely asynchronous nests. To test
this prediction, two other manipulations must be conducted: completely
asynchronous nests and completely synchronous nests.
Completely asynchronous nests will be created by fostering eggs onto
parents that are already incubating complete clutches. The eggs from
focal nests will be removed each morning as they are laid and replaced
with artificial surrogate eggs. The real eggs will be placed into
the nest of an incubating foster pair. To maintain the appropriate
clutch size, eggs will be removed from the foster parent’s nest and placed
into a third (dump) nest (not to be included in the study). As the
eggs hatch, they will be placed back into the original parent’s nest, removing
surrogate eggs appropriately. The end result will be a brood of nestlings
all of which hatched on different days (completely asynchronous brood).
To generate synchronous broods, unmanipulated
nests will be monitored until the first nestlings hatch. At that
point, the remaining unhatched eggs will be exchanged with hatch-day nestlings
adopted from other nests (donor nests – not to be included in analyses).
This will generate broods of young in which all of the nestlings hatched
on the same day. Under the egg viability hypothesis, these latter
nests should experience the greatest reproductive success. Reproductive
success will be measured as the number of young surviving to fledging and
as nestling mass on Day 12 (asymptotic mass). Kruskal-Wallis multiway
nonparametric ANOVA will be used to statistically evaluate the data.
Amundsen, T., and Slagsvold T. 1996. Lack’s brood reduction hypothesis
and avian
hatching asynchrony: what’s next? Oikos. 76:
613-620.
Anderson, D.J. 1990. Evolution of obligate siblicide in boobies.
1. A test of the
insurance-egg hypothesis. Am. Nat. 135: 334-350.
Anderson, S.J. 1995. The role of parents in sibilicidal brood
reduction of two booby
species. The Auk. 112: 860-869.
Aparicio, J.M. 1997. Costs and benefits of surplus offspring
in the lesser kestrel (Falco
naumanni). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 41: 129-137.
Clark, Anne B., and Wilson, David S. 1981. Avian breeding adaptations;
hatching
asynchrony, brood reduction, and nest failure.
Quarterly Review of Biology. 56:
253-277.
Deeming, D.C. and Ferguson, M.W.J. 1992. Physiological
effects of incubation
temperature on embryonic development in reptiles
and birds. In: Egg Incubation: its
effects on embryonic development in birds
and reptiles (eds. C.C. Deeming and
M.W.J. Ferguson), pp. 147-173. Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge.
Drent, R.H. 1975. Incubation. Avian Biology
5: 333-420.
Drummond, Hugh. 1987. Review of parent-offspring conflict and
brood reduction in the
pelicaniformes. Colonial Waterbirds. 10: 1-14.
Evans, R.M. 1990. Terminal egg neglect in the American white
pelican. Wilson Bull. 102:
684-692.
Evans, R.M., and McMahon, B.F. 1987. Within-brood variation in
growth and condition
in relation to brood reduction in the American
white pelican. Wilson Bull. 99: 190-201.
Ewert, M.A. 1992. Cold torpor, diapause, delayed hatching
and aestivation in reptiles
and birds. In: Egg Incubation: its effects
on embryonic development in birds and
reptiles (eds. C.C. Deeming and M.W.J. Ferguson),
pp. 173-191. Cambridge
University Press, Cambridge.
Hussell, D.J.T. 1985. On the adaptive basis for hatching
asynchrony: brood reduction,
nest failure and asynchronous hatching in
snow buntings. Ornis Scand. 16: 205-212.
Krebs, Elizabeth. 1999. Last but not least: nestling growth and
survival
in asynchronously
hatching crimson rosellas. Animal Ecology.
68: 266-281.
Magrath, Robert D. 1990. Hatching asynchrony in altricial birds.
Biol. Rev. 65: 587-622.
O’Connor, R.J. 1984. The Growth and Development of Birds.
John Wiley and Sons,
New York.
Rahn, H. 1991. Why birds lay eggs. In: Egg
Incubation: its effects on embryonic
development in birds and reptiles (eds. C.C.
Deeming and M.W.J. Ferguson), pp.
345-360. Cambridge University Press,
Cambridge.
Richter, Wayne. 1984. Nestling survival and growth in the yellow-headed
blackbird,
Xanthocephalus xanthocephalus. Ecology. 65:
597-608.
Ricklefs, R.E. 1995. Brood reduction in the curve-billed thrasher.
The Condor. 67:
505-510.
Rol’nik, V.V. 1970. Bird Embryology. Israeli Program
for Scientific Transactions,
Jerusalem.
Stoleson, S.H. and Beissinger, S.R. 1997. Hatching asynchrony,
brood reduction, and
food limitation in a neotropical parrot. Ecological
Monographs. 67; 131-154.
Stoleson, S.H. and Bessinger, S.R. 1999. Egg viability
as a constraint on hatching
synchrony at high ambient temperatures.
J. Anim. Ecol. 68: 951-962.
Stouffer, P.C. and Power, H.W. 1991. An experimental test of
the brood-reduction
hypothesis in European starlings. The Auk.
108: 519-531.
Strehl, Charles. 1978. Asynchrony of hatching in red-winged blackbirds
and survival of
late and early hatching birds. Wilson Bull.
90: 653-655.
Veiga, J.P. 1992. Hatching asynchrony in the house sparrow:
a test of the egg-viability
hypothesis. Am. Nat. 139: 669-675.
Veiga, J.P. and Vinuela, J. 1993. Hatching asynchrony and
hatching success in the house
sparrow: evidence for the egg viability hypothesis.
Ornis Scand. 24: 237-242.
Webb, D.R. 1987. Thermal tolerance of avian embryos: a
review. Condor 89: 874-898.
White, F.N. and Kinney, J.L. 1974. Avian incubation.
Science 186: 107-115.
Wiebe, K.L. 1996. The insurance-egg hypothesis and extra reproductive
value of last-
laid eggs in clutches of American kestrels.
The Auk. 113: 258-261.
Wilson, H.R. 1991. Physiological requirements of the developing
embryo: temperature
and turning. Poultry Science Symposium,
22: Avian Incubation (ed. S.G. Tullett), pp.
145-156. Butterworth-Heineman, London. |